Intertemporal consumption and lifecycle in a pandemic context: an experimental approximation

Authors

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.22267/rtend.242502.254

Keywords:

saving, economic behavior, consumer, experimental method, decision theory

Abstract

The Covid-19 pandemic generated uncertainty among consumers, a slowdown in consumption and an increase of the added saving at world level, the microeconomic evidence showed a tendency towards dissaving and growing consumption. These variations activate questioning about the consequences of confinement in intertemporal consumption, at the same time they allow to provide new empirical evidence about life-cycle models in their standard or neoclassical and behavioral versions. The purpose of this work was to experimentally evaluate the intertemporal consumption patterns from   the postulates of both life cycle models. To this end, an experimental simulation exercise of online purchases of commodities was carried out with the participation of 210 consumers who were subjected to treatments that included a baseline, income increase scenarios and no-income withdrawal scenarios. The results verified the existence of consistent responses with the behavioral model in 85% of the cases and with the standard model for the remaining 15%; that is, the tendencies to smooth consumption and increase savings were in the minority in the group evaluated and the confinement context did not translate into more self-controlled intertemporal consumption behaviors.

Downloads

Download data is not yet available.

Author Biographies

Claudia Milena Pico Bonilla, Institución Universitaria Politécnico Grancolombiano

P.h.D. in Psychology from the Universidad Nacional de Colombia, Professor at the Institución Universitaria Politécnico Grancolombiano. ORCID: 0000-0001-8839-5462. E-mail: cmpico@poligran.edu.co, Bogotá - Colombia.

Luis Eduardo Sandoval Garrido, Universidad Militar Nueva Granada

P.h.D. in Economics from the Universidad de Chile, Associate Professor at the Universidad Militar Nueva Granada, ORCID: 0000-0001-9615-6533. E-mail: luis.sandoval@unimilitar.edu.co, Bogotá – Colombia.

References

(1) Alessie, R. & de Ree, J. (2009). Explaining The Hump In Life Cycle Consumption profiles. De Economist, 157, 107-120. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10645-009-9119-4

(2) Alessie, R. Devereux, M. & Weber, G. (1997). Intertemporal Consumption, Durables and Liquidity Constraints: A Cohort Analysis, European Economic Review, 41(1), 37-59, https://doi.org/10.1016/0014-2921(95)00128-X

(3) Ando, A. & Modigliani, F. (2005). The life cycle hypothesis of saving: aggregate implications and tests. En F. Modigliani. (Ed.), The collected papers of Franco Modigliani. (pp. 62-78). MIT Press.

(4) Arslan, Y. Guler, B. & Taskin, T. (2021). Price search, consumption inequality and expenditure inequality over the life-cycle. International Economic Review, 62(1), 295-320. https://doi.org/10.1111/iere.12488

(5) Attanasio, O & Borella, M. (2006). Stochastic Components of Individual Consumption: A Time Series Analysis of Grouped Data. National Bureau of Economic Research, 12456, 1-48, https://doi.org/10.3386/w12456

(6) Attanasio, O. & Weber, G. (2010). Consumption and Saving: Models of Intertemporal Allocation and Their Implications for Public Policy. Journal of Economic Literature, 48(3), 693-751.

(7) Bancolombia. (2021). Comportamiento del gasto en los hogares: crecimiento durante 2021. Bancolombia. https://www.bancolombia.com/wps/portal/negocios/actualizate/perspectivas-economicas/comportamiento-gasto-hogares-2021.

(8) Binswanger, J. (2012). Life cycle saving: Insights from the perspective of bounded rationality. European Economic Review, 56(3), 605-623. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroecorev.2012.01.003

(9) Carbone, E. & Infante, G. (2014). Comparing behavior under risk and under ambiguity in a lifecycle experiment. Theory and Decision, 77, 312-322. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11238-014-9443-2

(10) Carbone, E. & Infante, G. (2015). Are groups better planners than individuals? An experimental análisis. Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Economics, 57, 112-119. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socec.2015.04.006

(11) Carroll, C. (1997). Buffer-stock saving and the life-cycle/permanent income. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 112(1), 1–55. https://doi.org/10.1162/003355397555109

(12) Castañeda, M. y Hernández, M. (2021). El nuevo consumidor colombiano: cambios y tendencias. BBVA. https://acortar.link/udXBHs

(13) Deaton, A. & Paxton, C. (1994). Intertemporal Choice and Inequality. Journal of Political Economy, 102(3), 437-467. https://doi.org/10.1086/261941

(14) Deaton, A. (1991). Saving and liquidity constraints. Econometrica, 59(5), 1221-1248. https://doi.org/10.2307/2938366

(15) Deaton, A. (2005). Franco Modigliani and the Life Cycle Theory of Consumption. SSRN.

(16) DiCrosta, A., Ceccato, I., Marchetti, D., La Malva, P., Maiella, R., Cannito, L., Cipi, M., Mammarella, N., Palumbo, R., Verrocchio, M., Palumbo, R. & Di Domenico, A. (2021). Psychological factors and consumer behavior during the COVID-19 pandemic. PLoS ONE, 16(1), 1-23. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0256095

(17) García, C. (2018). Ecuaciones de Euler del consumo: Recorrido histórico, evidencia empírica, y una interpretación de su persistencia en la macroeconomía moderna [Tesis de pregrado, Universidad del Valle]. Repositorio institucional. https://bibliotecadigital.univalle.edu.co/bitstream/10893/11243/1/3340-0582398-E.pdf

(18) Graham, F. & Isaac, A. (2002). The behavioral life-cycle theory of consumer behavior: survey evidence. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 48(4), 391-401. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0167-2681(01)00242-6

(19) Green, L., Fry, A. & Myerson, J. (1994). Discounting of delayed rewards: A life-span comparison. Psychological Science, 5(1), 33-36. https://doi.org/10.1111%2Fj.1467-9280.1994.tb00610.x

(20) Guglielminetti, E. & Rondinelli, C. (2021). Consumption and saving patterns in Italy during Covid-19. Questioni di Economia e Finanza, (620), 5-43. https://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3891608

(21) Jin, X., Zhao, Y., Song, W. & Zhao, T. (2021). Save for Safe: Effect of COVID-19 Pandemic on Consumers’ Saving and Spending Behavior in China. Frontiers in psychology, 12, 1-8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.636859

(22) Jørgensen, T. (2016). Euler equation estimation: Children and credit constraints. Quantitative Economics, 7, 935-968. https://doi.org/10.3982/QE492

(23) Keynes, J.M. (1965). Teoría general de la ocupación, el interés y el dinero (E. Hornedo, Trad., Séptima Edición) Fondo de Cultura Económica. (Trabajo original publicado en 1936).

(24) Koehler, D., Langstaff, J. & Liu, W.Q. (2015). A simulated financial savings task for studying consumption and retirement decision making. Journal of Economic Psychology, 46, 89-97. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2014.12.004

(25) Laibson, D. & Harris, C. (2001). Dynamic choices of hyperbolic consumers. Econometrica, 69(4), 443-477. https://doi.org/10.1111/1468-0262.00225

(26) Lera, F. (1996). Teorías macroeconómicas explicativas del ahorro de las economías domésticas: situación actual de debate. Are assets fungible?: Testing the behavioral theory of life-cycle savings. Cuadernos de economía, 24, 91-117.

(27) Levin, L. (1998). Are assets fungible?: Testing the behavioral theory of life-cycle savings. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 36(1), 59-83. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0167-2681(98)00070-5

(28) Menger, C. (2007). Principles of political economy. Ludwig Von Mises Institute. https://cdn.mises.org/Principles%20of%20Economics_5.pdf

(29) Modigliani, F. y Brumberg, R. (1954). Utility analysis and the consumption function: an interpretation of cross-section data. En K. Kurihara. (Ed.), Post- Keynesian Economic (pp. 388–436). Rutgers University Press.

(30) Pico, C. (2021). La teoría económica y el modelo de perspectiva conductual: una aproximación experimental al efecto de los ingresos y precios sobre la elección del consumidor [Tesis doctoral, Universidad Nacional de Colombia]. Repositorio Institucional UNAL . https://repositorio.unal.edu.co/handle/unal/80026

(31) Quintana-Domeque, C. & Wohlfart, J. (2016). “Relative concerns for consumption at the top”: An intertemporal analysis for the UK. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 129, 172-194. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2016.06.005

(32) Shefrin, H. & Thaler, R. (1988). The behavioral life-cycle hypothesis. Economic inquiry, 26(4) , 609-643. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1465-7295.1988.tb01520.x

(33) Srivastava, R. & Bhanot, S. (2022). Study on the impact of COVID‐19 on the purchase and mental behaviour of Indian consumers during lockdown. International Social Science Journal, 72(244), 437-459. https://doi.org/10.1111%2Fissj.12329

(34) Statman, M. (2017). Standard and Behavioral Life-Cycle Theories and Public Policy . The Journal of Retirement, 5(2), 12-25. https://doi.org/10.3905/jor.2017.5.2.012

(35) Thaler, R. (2008). Mental Accounting and Consumer Choice. Marketing Science, 27(1), 15-25. https://doi.org/10.1287/mksc.1070.0330

(36) Tobin, J. (1967). Life-cycle saving and balanced growth. En W. Fellner. (Ed.), Ten Economic Essays in the Tradition of Irving Fisher (pp. 231-256). Wiley.

(37) Tversky, A. & Kahneman, D. (1981). The Framing of Decisions and the Psychology of Choice. Science, 211(4481), 453-458. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.7455683

(38) White, B. B. (1978). Empirical tests of the life cycle hypothesis. The American Economic Review, 68(4), 547-560.

Published

2024-07-01

How to Cite

Pico Bonilla, C. M., & Sandoval Garrido, L. E. (2024). Intertemporal consumption and lifecycle in a pandemic context: an experimental approximation. Tendencias, 25(2), 57–85. https://doi.org/10.22267/rtend.242502.254